Tuning the infant brain – in our time

Tuning the infant brain – in our time
08 March 2010

Evidence of a developing consistency in attitude among neuroscientists and those economists and children and families policy makers who have been won over by the case for early intervention was audible on BBC radio, last week.

One key connecting term is “regulation,” the importance of which was discussed in the latest BBC Radio 4 In Our Time broadcast and podcast on the infant brain and the psychology of early childhood.

“Emotional regulation” is a term increasingly familiar in the vocabulary of social and emotional learning programs. It has figured particularly prominently in the findings of psychologist Joseph Durlak and colleagues at Loyola University, Chicago.

Their systematic review of the territory in 2008 indicated that wherever social and emotional regulation was part of the school curriculum, children tended to be “happier and better behaved and to perform better academically”.

Self regulation figures similarly in US Prevention Research Center founder Mark Greenberg’s interest in mindfulness and in Tom Dishion’s studies of the ecology of childhood. "If we can influence the way the home, schools and neighborhood change children's behavior and health,” Dishion has argued, “then we can begin to have significant impacts on well-being". [See, for example: Bringing prevention services home to the family]

The BBC broadcast traced the evolution of ideas about development in infancy through the theories of the child psychologist Jean Piaget, and via the social linguist Noam Chomsky who argued that all humans are born in possession of an innate, universally applicable grammar.

Of the thinking since Chomsky, Usha Goswami, Director of the Centre for Neuroscience in Education at the University of Cambridge said: “I think what’s fascinating and which is really different from what constructivists like Piaget thought is how many of the ways that the infant and young child’s brain functions are actually very similar to what happens in adulthood.

“So, for example, it used to be thought that children didn’t reason logically - not very young children, not toddlers and not four-year-olds - but now it’s realized that children can be just as logical as adults, but they just have a lot less knowledge.

“So sometimes they’ll make inferences that are incorrect – and what seems to be so important developmentally is the overall regulation of your system. So it’s having enough experience, so that you learn all these extra contextual factors that help constrain reasoning, but it’s also being able to self regulate your emotions, being able to function effectively in the face of conflict or other hindrances to pure logic that are very important developmentally.

“So it’s not so much the content of the mind but how efficient you can be with the content of your own mind, given all the other things that you’ve got to do as well.”

Of the crucial relationship between the innate and the world “learned” in infancy, Denis Mareschal, Professor of Psychology at the Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development at Birkbeck College, University of London said: ”The essential building blocks were understood 100 years ago. What we are reappraising is the real complexity of the vast networks that are present in the brain and how just from a few very simple building blocks we can get the range of complex behaviors that we observe in adults.

“There doesn’t seem to be a magic bullet. There isn’t some aspect of the human brain which is present only in humans and not in other animals. We don’t have the biggest brains; we don’t have the largest cortex. We don’t have the best brain metabolism and so forth.…

“So there must be something else – and that something else is a complex interaction between our neural abilities - and the fact that, compared to precocious animals, we have very extensive developmental periods. In infancy, our brains our being tuned to the environment.”

[See also: It’s time to sweat the early intervention asset]

Explainers

Tom Dishion

Tom Dishion is Director of the Child and Family Center and Professor of Psychology at the University of Oregon. Previously a Research Scientists at the Oregon Social Learning Center, Dishion is responsible for ground breaking research on the way adolescent peer groups escalate substance use, delinquency, and violence. His program design and clinical work focus on family-centered interventions. He was the 2007 Bennett Lecturer in Prevention Science at Penn State University.

Mark Greenberg

Mark Greenberg is the Edna Peterson Bennett Endowed Chair in Prevention Research and Director of the Prevention Research Center for the Promotion of Human Development at Penn State University. PRC's mix of collaborative research and evaluation, training, technical assistance and policy-relevant information on best practices has led to significant contributions to science, Pennsylvania communities and to federal, state and local government. Mark Greenberg is a Board Member of Prevention Action.

Joseph Durlak

Joseph Durlak is Professor of Clinical Psychology at Loyola University, Chicago, where his primary research interest is in prevention and promotion programs for children and adolescents.

He has collaborated with Roger Weissberg at the University of Illinois at Chicago on a three-year grant to conduct a meta-analysis of positive youth development (PYD) programs. They hold a database of approximately 700 interventions targeting children and adolescents between the ages of five and 18 and have conducted reviews of universal school- after school- and family-based programs. He invites correspondence from students who would like to learn how to use meta-analysis to evaluate a research literature.

Search form

Advertise here

Subscribe to our newsletter

Click here to subscribe to the Prevention Action Newsletter.

Monthly archive

Editor's Picks

There is more to the international transfer of prevention programs than just hitting the “copy and paste” buttons. The introduction of the Big Brothers Big Sisters mentoring program to Ireland offers insights into how to succeed.

Few people working with children will have heard the term “prevention scientist,” let alone know what one is or does. Yet this relatively new breed of researcher is behind the growing list of evidence-based programs being promoted in western developed countries. A new publication puts them under the microscope.

Crime and antisocial behavior prevention efforts have flourished over the last 10 years in the US. This progress can and should be used to help communities improve the life chances of their young people, a recent update urges.

Given the well-known barriers to implementing evidence-based programs, is it better to identify their discrete elements and trust practitioners to combine them in tailored packages depending on the needs of the child and family in question?

The final official review on child protection offers a shakeup of services.